Introduction
With the shift in education
¡°from an interest in cognitive and developmental
theories of learning to a social and collaborative
view of learning¡± (Ortega, 1997:82; cf, Warschauer,
1997) and the gradual integration of computer
technology in classrooms, synchronous and asynchronous
computer-mediated communication (for definitions,
see Warschauer & Healey, 1998; Burdeau,
1997) has been more and more widely used in
language learning classrooms, as well as virtually
across all disciplines. Among the various forms
of synchronous, ¡®real-time¡¯ communication in
language classrooms is one particular kind,
which is called computer-assisted classroom
discussion (CACD), which use special software
programs for local area networks, such as Daedalus
InterChange by Daedalus Inc.
When using programs like InterChange,
each student sits in front of a computer terminal.
The computer screen is divided into two halves.
The student composes messages on the bottom
half, and when ready, hits the send button.
The message appears immediately on the upper
half of every participant¡¯s screen, which also
contains all the entries posted by all the participants.
The screen is scrollable, so that it is very
easy for students to read or re-read carefully
what others have been talking about. The entries
are listed in chronological order, with identifiable
names of the senders and the time code of each
entry. The participants can read each others¡¯
entries at their own pace on the upper screen
and type in messages at their own leisure on
the lower screen without being interfered by
the incoming messages. This kind of programs
have introduced the possibility of real-time,
synchronous, many-to-many written discussion
used either in a whole class or by small groups
within the class (Huang, 1998; Chun, 1994; Warschauer,
1996a; Kern, 1995).
Research on the use of computer-assisted
classroom discussion for language teaching and
learning has focused on the general benefits
or advantages of CACD. These include questions
of learners¡¯ motivation, participation, language-production.
This paper reviews these relevant findings.
1.
Learners¡¯ motivation 
In order to test the much alleged
motivational benefits of SLA students¡¯ use of
CMC, Warschauer (1996b) surveyed 167 university
ESL and EFL students in 12 university writing
courses internationally (in the U.S., Hong Kong
and Taiwan). It was found that students overall
had a positive attitude toward using computers
and this attitude was consistent across a number
of variables, including gender, typing skill,
and access to a computer at home. Further analysis
showed that two variables in particular relating
to computer experience, self-reported knowledge
of computer and amount of experience using electronic
mail, correlated positively with student motivation.
From a factor analysis of the
survey, Warschauer found that factors that influenced
students¡¯ positive attitude toward computers
included the following three aspects. The strongest
factor was the perceived benefits of communicating
via computer, which seemed to include ¡°feeling
part of a community, developing thoughts and
ideas, learning about different people and cultures,
and students¡¯ learning from each other¡± (Warschauer,
1996b). The second factor he analysed was the
feeling of personal empowerment coming from
using computers. This involved issues such as
¡°enhancing personal power, overcoming isolation,
and making it less threatening to contact people¡±
(Warschauer, 1996b). The third factor involved
the perceived enhancement of learning opportunities
which arose from using computers. Warschauer
(1996b) found,
¡°Using computers, they feel
can learn faster, become more creative, and
write better essays. They feel they have more
control of their learning and more opportunities
to practise English¡±.
Warschauer also identified another
factor, which he named ¡®achievement¡¯. Interestingly,
it seemed not to be any gain or achievement
closely related to the curriculum or course;
rather more to do with students¡¯ idea of career
or life-long ability. It was more like the instrumental
benefits and students¡¯ perceived intrinsic satisfaction
of accomplishment of their personal power, in
Warschauer¡¯s own word, ¡®empowerment¡¯ (1996b).
The only distinction Warschauer made between
these two factors seem to be that the later
is more involved with learners¡¯ affective perception.
A careful examination of Warschauer¡¯s
motivational study would reveal that the No.1
highest score item in his 30-question survey
was ¡°learning how to use computers is
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Survey Questions Mean
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Learning how to use computers is important for
my career 4.437
I enjoy seeing the things I write printed out
3.994
I enjoy using the computer to communicate with
people around the world 3.982
An advantage of email is you can contact people
anytime you want 3.976
Using email and the Internet is good way to
learn more about different people and cultures
3.976
Computers help people overcome weakness and
powerlessness 3.939
I am less afraid to contact people by email
than in person 3.928
--------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Warschauer¡¯s highest mean-score items
Important in my career (4.437)¡±.
One can expect such a response not only from
language students, but from any students, indeed
most people in any discipline, any field, with
today¡¯s wide spread use of all sorts of electronic
means of communication, synchronous or asynchronous.
One could argue, indeed, most of his highest
mean score items are not language learner specific,
therefore, the fact that students like using
computer for communication can not convincingly
be said to show that using computers can in
someway enhance learners¡¯ language learning
experience. It certainly does not tell us in
what way the learners¡¯ learning is enhanced.
2.
Learners¡¯ participation 
A generally-accepted finding
of CACD literature is that CACD discussion generates
more equal participation from all participants
(Kern, 1995; Chun, 1994; Beauvois, 1992; McComb,
1993). There may be some reasons for this equalising
power of CACD. Ortega (1997) attributes the
more equal participation pattern to the following
two features of CACD:
¡°(a) Interactants are less apprehensive
about being evaluated by interlocuters, and
thus more willing to participate at their leisure;
and (b) they are less affected by wait time,
turn-taking, and other elements of traditional
interaction, enabling them to participate as
much as they want, whenever they want, with
opportunities for contribution being more equally
distributed among participants¡± (p.85).
The equalising feature of CACD,
researchers find, includes the following three
aspects. First, there is a more democratic distribution
of conversational power, hence, the traditional
teacher-fronted, teacher-dominant classroom
can give to a more student-centred, less teacher-authoritative
conversation pattern (Kelm, 1992; Kern, 1995).
In these studies, the teacher is merely another
participant in the discussion, contributing
to the CACD discussion the similar way as the
other learners do. ¡°The traditional figure of
the teacher as authority source and expert is
subverted in that the role of the teacher during
the electronic discussion is that of a mere
participant¡± (Ortega, 1997: 85). After careful
analysis of his data of second semester French
four-skill course, Kern (1995) finds that the
style and organisation of CACD discussion has
an important effect on student-teacher dynamics,
and that the instructors do not ¡°occupy a focal
position in the Interchange sessions¡± (Ortega,
1997:79), and that ¡°teacher control is compromised¡±
(p.79).
The second aspect of the equal
participation pattern is closely related to
the first one. Due to the changes in teacher
role, students during CACD sessions assume more
control and more responsibility for the electronic
discussions. It seems that talking to each other,
rather than asking (more frequently answering)
teacher¡¯s display questions, students during
electronic discussion were able to ¡°express
themselves freely, comfortably, and creatively¡±
(Warschater, 1996a:16). Further, according to
Warschauer,
¡°participating in electronic discussion assisted
their thinking ability, and that they (participants)
did not feel stress during electronic discussion¡±
(ibid, p.16).
The third, and probably the
most frequently addressed feature of students¡¯
participation pattern in electronic discussion
is the equal distribution of turns among participants.
Researchers (Kelm, 1992; Kern, 1995; Huang,
1998; Warschauer, 1996a) find
¡°all speakers share the floor
more equally, and students that do not normally
participate much in traditional classroom discussion
seem to dramatically increase their participation
in the electronic mode¡± (Ortega,1997:85).
Among L1 education literature,
Bump (1990) found that the level of participation
of English literature students in InterChange
sessions increased and this increase was particularly
obvious among students who were normally categorised
shy and reticent in oral discussions. This increase
was measured by turns students took during electronic
discussions. He attributed this increase of
participation among shy students to the fact
that there was no pressure on students before
they responded to any of others¡¯ postings, that
they were not interrupted by, nor interrupting
anyone while they tried to contribute to the
discussion, and that the anonimity of comments
sometimes also had a facilitating effect.
Studies in the foreign/second
language context also provide evidence for an
equal participation pattern among FL/SL students.
Kelm (1992), Beauvois (1992) and Kern (1995)
explored the use of InterChange in Portuguese
and French classes. The former two found that
students who were supposed to be shy, with low-motivation
and labelled as unsuccessful, and who were less
willing to participate in traditional teacher-led
oral discussions, increased their participation
greatly during CACD discussions. In her summary
of these benefits of CACD, Beauvois (1997:64)
maintains,
¡°students who are shy or otherwise
inhibited in the traditional classroom state
they find a ¡®voice¡¯ on the computer and tend
to participate more. In fact, there is usually
100% participation in the lab sessions¡± (see
Beauvois, 1992; Kelm, 1992; Kern, 1995).
The fact that most of these
studies have been conducted in whole class setting,
may have decreased the credibility and validity
of the conclusion because most of the normal
classroom practice of oral discussion is done
in small groups. Therefore, at least in comparing
with normal language teaching classroom-based
discussion, studies of participation patterns
of CACD discussions should be more informative
if the CACD is conducted in small groups. Warschauer
(1996a) and Huang (1998) are among the few researchers
who compared small group interactions in both
face-to-face and CACD discussions. Their findings
provide some further evidence that CACD generates
more equal participation patterns among participants
within small groups. Warschauer (1996a) compared
open-ended discussions in both face-to-face
and electronic mode by four groups of four participants
each. He found that three of the four groups
showed ¡°substantially¡± (Warschauer, 1996a: 20)
greater equality of participation during CACD
sessions. Huang (1998) also found that ¡°on the
computer, students participated in prewriting
discussions equally, while in face-to-face context,
some students participated more than the others¡±.
To sum up, this equal participation
pattern of CACD can be attributed to the following
aspects that CACD discussions present participants.
¡¤Less anxiety
¡°InterChange is probably the
single most helpful aspect of the Daedalus system,
in that it vastly improves relations in the
classroom by creating a non-threatening and
supportive forum for the exchanges of ideas,
which seems to greatly improve both the students
morale and their willingness to participate¡±
(Butler, 1993).
¡°Students reported feeling that
they could express themselves freely, comfortably,
and creatively during electronic discussion,
that participating in electronic discussion
assisted their thinking ability, and that they
did not feel stress during electronic discussion¡±
(Warschauer, 1996a:16).
¡¤More control
Participants can choose when,
what and who to ¡° talk¡± to. ¡°They could take
as much time as necessary to compose their responses
before posting then to the whole class discussion.
I expected InterChange to encourage some of
the quieter participants to speak their minds
more forcefully than they did in oral class
discussion. And they did¡± (Hughes, 1994). Students
can also choose and change their own topics
and negotiate more freely on how to proceed.
¡¤Authenticity of tasks
Rather than the IRF (initiate,
response and feedback) (Mehan, 1985) interaction
pattern in traditional classroom discourse,
in CACD sessions
¡°Students are actively writing,
practising and receiving immediate feedback.
When communicating to an authentic audience
of their peers, students are more likely to
write for a real purpose, to take ownership
of their writing and to articulate their ideas
fully. Such writing experience prepares students
for careers in business and many other fields,
including academia, in which wide area networks
are increasingly used both for discussion and
collaborative writing projects¡± (Sommers, 1997).
3.
Learners¡¯ production 
In SL/FL teaching and learning
contexts, with the emphasis on the use of the
language rather than the form of it, students¡¯
communicating and interacting in their target
language has become more and more the focus
of most language instructors. Learners¡¯ production
of the target language, especially, of ¡°comprehensible
output¡± (Swain, 1985), is believed to be able
to push their interlanguage system one step
further in its development. Researchers (cf.
Kern, 1995) have found that CACD discussions
provide learners with opportunities to be engaged
in meaningful interaction and with authentic
purposes, and that these opportunities for learner
output are more abundant than in traditional
face-to-face settings.
There is also unanimous belief
that the language produced by participants during
CACD sessions belongs to a hybrid language variety,
which displays both characteristics of written
and oral language. Hence, it is hoped that ¡°the
written competence from CACD can gradually be
transferred to the students¡¯ speaking competence
as well¡± (Chun, 1994:29). CACD may well be an
effective forum for facilitating the acquisition
of the various components of communicative competence.
Ferrara, Brunner and Whittemore (1991) term
it as ¡®interactive written discourse (IWD).
They maintain that IWD
¡°is a hybrid register that resembles
both speech and writing (displaying characteristics
of both oral and written language), and yet
is neither¡± (p.10).
This ¡°hybrid¡± can be shown through
¡°features of heavy involvement (e.g., deictics,
adverbs of time, and direct questions) traditionally
associated with oral language and face-to-face
interaction¡± (Ferrara, Brunner & Whittemore,
1991:22) and the fact of language being edited
and written (ibid: 26). When noting students¡¯
indifference to the appropriate usage of the
target language, Beauvois (1992) concludes that
¡°computer-assisted classroom
discussion is neither really speaking nor is
it exactly writing. The problems that arise
are therefore of a different nature from those
common to the spoken or written domains¡± (p.
460).
Baym (1995) also points out
that
¡°CMC has generally been understood
through a ¡®talk¡¯ metaphor, which compares on-line
interaction to face-to-face conversation. While
CMC is obviously not talk, it is in many ways
more like conversation than writing because
it is interactive, relatively spontaneous and
generally unplanned.¡±
Drawing from the above researchers,
it appears safe to say that the language produced
by participants during CACD sessions has its
own unique characteristics which are similar
in some aspects to those of oral language and
in some other aspects to written language, and
yet are not totally oral, nor written. What
is needed is more research identifying exactly
what characteristics this IWD as a unique language
register have.
CACD literature about learners¡¯
language output has focused on both quantity
and quality of the language participants produce
through the networked computers during CACD
discussions. Some researchers find that CACD
discussions provide more opportunities for communicative
practice of the target language and more opportunities
for learners to produce meaningful output than
traditional L2 classroom practice, thus resulting
in an increase in the amount of participation,
hence an increase in participants¡¯ language
output.
In his study, Kern (1995) compared
electronic and face-to-face discussion of the
same length in his university French class and
concluded that ¡°there were striking differences
in quantity of production between InterChange
and oral discussions¡± (p.464). Students had
from two to three and a half times more turns
and produced two to four times more sentences
in the InterChange sessions than in the oral
discussion. He also found that
¡°a less marked, but substantial,
difference is also shown in the number of words
produced (216 to 316 average words per student
in the InterChange discussions vs. 111 to 137
average words per student in the oral discussions)¡±
(Kern, 1995:465).
There are also different findings
in terms of whether CACD increases the total
amount of learner output. Huang (1998) did a
comparative study of her EFL students in both
CACD and oral discussions. She found that
¡°in groups that consisted of
four members, the FF (face-to-face) discussions
produced four times more speech than the CM
(computer-mediated) ones. There did not seem
to be much change in the speech production rate
over time within either context.¡±
Students in her FF sessions
produced 108.1 words per minute while those
in CM sessions did only 27.3 words per minute.
Fortunately, studies reporting less language
production in CACD are fewer than those reporting
increases in quantity of language output.
On the other hand, other studies anecdotally
reported dramatic increase in students¡¯ language
output (Beauvois, 1992; Bump, 1990; Kelm, 1992).
So far, then, it seems still unclear whether
or not CACD generates an increase in learners¡¯
productivity. Even if it does, it is difficult
to say whether or not this increase in the total
amount of learners¡¯ output facilitates or enhances
learners¡¯ language learning experience. Learners¡¯
output has to be carefully examined to see whether
or not learners ¡®push¡¯ their present linguistic
repertoire and extend any components of communicative
competence. It has to be determined that learners
are not merely practising what they have already
known well enough (Fernquest, personal communication
through emails, July, 1998).
Naturally, researchers have
also devoted much of their attention to the
quality of the language output learners produce
during CACD sessions. Besides the characteristic
of a different register discussed previously,
researchers have also looked at the quality
of CACD discourse in terms of the length of
turns, the length of sentences, and the complexity
of sentences (Kern, 1995; Kelm, 1992; Warschauer,
1996a; Chun, 1994).
It has been reported that CACD
discussions yield discourse of a better quality.
Researchers have compared face-to-face and electronic
discussions and some find that CACD discussions
produce longer turns than face-to-face ones,
and that students in CACD sessions use language
that is both more formal and more complex, both
lexically and syntactically (Warschauer, 1996a).
CACD participants use language covering a wide
range of discourse and communicative functions
(Chun, 1994; Kern, 1995).
¡° Learners are definitely taking
the initiative, constructing and expanding on
topics, taking a more active role in discourse
management than is typically found in normal
classroom discussion. In addition, learners
exhibit the ability to give feedback to others,
as well as sociolinguistic competence in greeting
and leave-taking, requesting confirmation or
clarification, and apologizing¡± (Chun, 1994:28).
Kelm (1992) finds that his Portuguese
students¡¯ accuracy is greatly improved in CACD
sessions. He notes that certain grammatical
errors, like incorrect uses of gerunds and progressives,
were greatly reduced at an 80% rate.
There are also, however, some
conflicting findings in terms of the complexity
of the language used by participants in CACD
sessions. In contrast to Warschauer¡¯s (1996a)
finding, Kern (1995) and Chun (1994) find that
participants during electronic discussions produce
a higher proportion of simple sentences than
complex ones. On the other hand, Kern (1995)
also concludes that
¡°students¡¯ language output in
InterChange was of an overall greater level
of sophistication than in oral discussion, in
terms of the range of its morphosyntactic features
and in terms of the variety of discourse functions
expressed¡± (p. 470).
This paper has made a brief
review of CACD literature in terms of learner
motivation, learner participation and learner
production. It is aimed to draw a general picture
of what we know, so far, about CACD participants
and their interaction. As can be seen from the
above discussion, if one accepts that interaction,
especially, negotiation of input message meaning
between learners is relevant to SLA processes
and conducive to interlanguage development,
one is forced to conclude that there is a general
lack of concern in these studies about the interactional
features of CACD discourse (as was pointed out
in relative studies by Kern, 1995; Chun, 1994;
Kelm, 1992; Warschauer, 1996a; Huang, 1998).
Further investigations of such electronic discussions
will need also to consider the task-specific
context in CACD lab. It may be that different
tasks will bring about different interactional
discourse features. It is to this end, that
a specific CACD corpus is examined in another
paper titled ¡°Language functional features identified
in a CACD corpus¡±.
Bibliography

Baym, N. K. 1995. The Performance
of Humour in Computer-mediated Communication.
JCMC (on-line), Vol. 1, No. 2, URL-http://www.comm.wayne.edu/NancyBaym.html
Beauvois, M. H. 1992. Computer-Assisted
Classroom Discussion in the Foreign Language
Classroom: Conversation in Slow Motion. Foreign
Languages Annals, 25 (5), pp.455-464
Beauvois, M. H. 1997. High-Tech,
High-Touch: From Discussion to Composition in
the Networked Classroom. Computer-Assisted Language
Learning, Vol. 10, No, 1, pp.57-70
Bump, J. 1990. Radical Changes
in Class Discussion Using Networked Computers.
Computers and Humanities, 24:49-65
Burdeau, I. 1997. Virtual Classrooms,
Virtual Schools: The New Possibilities, Paradigms
and Pedagogy Created by Synchronous Computer-
mediated Communication. Unpublished master¡¯s
thesis. University of Brighton
Butler, K. 1993. Daedalus, in
France: Teaching the Art of the English Essay.
Wings (on-line), Vol. 1, No. 2, URL-http://kairos,daedalus.com/wings/butler(k).1.2.html
Chun, D. M. 1994. Using Computer
Networking to Facilitate the Acquisition of
Interactive Competence. System, Vol. 22, No.
1, pp.17-31
Ferrara, K., Brunner, H. and
Whittemore, G. 1991. Interactive Written Discourse
as an Emergent Register. Written Communication,
Vol. 8, No. 1, pp. 8-34
Huang, Sue-yue. 1998. ¡° ?¡± Seminar
in the Language Centre, University of Brighton.
The full report will be submitted in November,
titled as ¡°Networked Computers Used in Pre-writing
Discussions and Peer Review Sessions: An Evaluation
of Several Types of LAN Software¡±, a report
for the project (No. 36042F), between Sept.
16, 1997 and Sept. 15, 1998, sponsored by the
National Science Council of the Taiwan government
Hughes, Emily, 1994. Voices
We Hear and Voices We Silence: Galloping through
InterChange. Wings (on-line), Vol. 2, No. 1,
URL-http://kairos,daedalus.com/wings/Hughes.2.1.html
Kelm, O. 1992. The Use of Synchronous
Computer Networks in Second Language Instruction:
A Preliminary Report. Foreign Language Annals,
25 (5), pp. 441-454
McComb, Man 1993. Augmenting
a Group Discussion Course with Computer-mediated
Communication in a Small College Setting. Interpersonal
Computing and Technology: An Electronic Journal
for the 21st Century (on-line), 1 (3). URL-http://www.helsinki.fi/science/optek/1993/n3/mccomb.txt
Mehan, H. 1985. The Structure
of Classroom Discourse. In T. A. van Dijk (ED),
Handbook of Discourse Analysis. London: Academic.
Pp. 120-131
Ortega, L. 1997. Processes and
Outcomes in Networked Classroom Interaction:
Defining the Research Agenda for L2 Computer-Assisted
Classroom Discussion. Language teaching and
Technology (on-line). URL-http://polyglot.cal.msu.edu/llt/vol1num1/ortega/default.html
Sommers, E. 1997. Hearing the
Students¡¯ Voices: A Daedalus Convert Goes National.
URL-http://kairos.daedalus.com/
Swain, M. 1985. Communicative
Competence: Some Roles of Comprehensible Input
and Comprehensible Output in its Development.
In Gass, S. M. & Madden, C. G. (Eds), Input
in Second Language Acquisition. Rowley, MA:
Newbury House. pp.235-253
Warschauer, M. & Healey,
D. 1998. Computers and Language Learning: An
Overview. Language Teaching, 31, pp.57-71
Warschauer, M. 1996a. Comparing
Face-to-face and Electronic Discussion in the
Second Language Classroom. CALICO Journal, Vol.
13, No.s 2 & 3,pp.7-26
Warschauer, M. 1996b. Motivational
Aspects of Using Computers for Writing and Communication.
URL-http://www.lll.hawaii.edu/nflrc/Networks/NW1/NW01.html
Warschauer, M. 1997. Computer-mediated
Collaborative Learning: Theory and Practice.
The Modern Language journal, Vol. 81, No. 4,
pp.470-487
|